OpenAlex Citation Counts

OpenAlex Citations Logo

OpenAlex is a bibliographic catalogue of scientific papers, authors and institutions accessible in open access mode, named after the Library of Alexandria. It's citation coverage is excellent and I hope you will find utility in this listing of citing articles!

If you click the article title, you'll navigate to the article, as listed in CrossRef. If you click the Open Access links, you'll navigate to the "best Open Access location". Clicking the citation count will open this listing for that article. Lastly at the bottom of the page, you'll find basic pagination options.

Requested Article:

What is replication?
Brian A. Nosek, Timothy M. Errington
PLoS Biology (2020) Vol. 18, Iss. 3, pp. e3000691-e3000691
Open Access | Times Cited: 306

Showing 1-25 of 306 citing articles:

Replicability, Robustness, and Reproducibility in Psychological Science
Brian A. Nosek, Tom E Hardwicke, Hannah Moshontz, et al.
Annual Review of Psychology (2021) Vol. 73, Iss. 1, pp. 719-748
Open Access | Times Cited: 465

Investigating the replicability of preclinical cancer biology
Timothy M. Errington, Maya B Mathur, Courtney K. Soderberg, et al.
eLife (2021) Vol. 10
Open Access | Times Cited: 154

Challenges for assessing replicability in preclinical cancer biology
Timothy M. Errington, Alexandria Denis, Nicole Perfito, et al.
eLife (2021) Vol. 10
Open Access | Times Cited: 113

Low availability of code in ecology: A call for urgent action
Antica Čulina, Ilona van den Berg, Simon Evans, et al.
PLoS Biology (2020) Vol. 18, Iss. 7, pp. e3000763-e3000763
Open Access | Times Cited: 111

Replication is important for educational psychology: Recent developments and key issues
Jonathan A. Plucker, Matthew C. Makel
Educational Psychologist (2021) Vol. 56, Iss. 2, pp. 90-100
Open Access | Times Cited: 103

Replicability, Robustness, and Reproducibility in Psychological Science
Brian A. Nosek, Tom E Hardwicke, Hannah Moshontz, et al.
(2021)
Open Access | Times Cited: 88

Validation of cluster analysis results on validation data: A systematic framework
Theresa Ullmann, Christian Hennig, Anne‐Laure Boulesteix
Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Data Mining and Knowledge Discovery (2021) Vol. 12, Iss. 3
Open Access | Times Cited: 81

Many Labs 5: Testing Pre-Data-Collection Peer Review as an Intervention to Increase Replicability
Charles R. Ebersole, Maya B Mathur, Erica Baranski, et al.
Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science (2020) Vol. 3, Iss. 3, pp. 309-331
Open Access | Times Cited: 78

Introduction to the EQIPD quality system
Anton Bespalov, René Bernard, Anja Gilis, et al.
eLife (2021) Vol. 10
Open Access | Times Cited: 62

Kinds of Replication: Examining the Meanings of “Conceptual Replication” and “Direct Replication”
Maarten Derksen, Jill G. Morawski
Perspectives on Psychological Science (2022) Vol. 17, Iss. 5, pp. 1490-1505
Open Access | Times Cited: 56

Replication concerns in sports and exercise science: a narrative review of selected methodological issues in the field
Cristian Mesquida, Jennifer Murphy, Daniël Lakens, et al.
Royal Society Open Science (2022) Vol. 9, Iss. 12
Open Access | Times Cited: 41

High replicability of newly discovered social-behavioural findings is achievable
John Protzko, Jon A. Krosnick, Leif D. Nelson, et al.
Nature Human Behaviour (2023) Vol. 8, Iss. 2, pp. 311-319
Open Access | Times Cited: 41

A Multilab Replication of the Induced-Compliance Paradigm of Cognitive Dissonance
David C. Vaidis, Willem W. A. Sleegers, Florian van Leeuwen, et al.
Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science (2024) Vol. 7, Iss. 1
Open Access | Times Cited: 13

A guide for social science journal editors on easing into open science
Priya Silverstein, Colin Elman, Amanda Kay Montoya, et al.
Research Integrity and Peer Review (2024) Vol. 9, Iss. 1
Open Access | Times Cited: 11

The relationship between performance validity testing, external incentives, and cognitive functioning in long COVID
Douglas M. Whiteside, Michael R. Basso, Chen Shen, et al.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology (2024) Vol. 46, Iss. 1, pp. 6-15
Closed Access | Times Cited: 9

Forty years in the making: A systematic review of the megatrends literature
Claire K. Naughtin, Emma Schleiger, Alexandra Bratanova, et al.
Futures (2024) Vol. 157, pp. 103329-103329
Open Access | Times Cited: 8

High Replicability of Newly-Discovered Social-behavioral Findings is Achievable
John Protzko, Jon A. Krosnick, Leif D. Nelson, et al.
(2020)
Open Access | Times Cited: 56

Deciding what to replicate: A decision model for replication study selection under resource and knowledge constraints.
Peder Mortvedt Isager, Robbie C. M. van Aert, Štěpán Bahník, et al.
Psychological Methods (2021) Vol. 28, Iss. 2, pp. 438-451
Open Access | Times Cited: 55

Ocean acidification drives global reshuffling of ecological communities
Ivan Nagelkerken, Sean D. Connell
Global Change Biology (2022) Vol. 28, Iss. 23, pp. 7038-7048
Open Access | Times Cited: 29

Temporal validity as meta-science
Kevin Munger
Research & Politics (2023) Vol. 10, Iss. 3
Open Access | Times Cited: 17

The Importance of Replication
Jon Grahe, Kelly Cuccolo
Cambridge University Press eBooks (2024), pp. 283-304
Closed Access | Times Cited: 7

A framework for evaluating reproducibility and replicability in economics
Anna Dreber, Magnus Johannesson
Economic Inquiry (2024)
Open Access | Times Cited: 6

The best time to argue about what a replication means? Before you do it
Brian A. Nosek, Timothy M. Errington
Nature (2020) Vol. 583, Iss. 7817, pp. 518-520
Closed Access | Times Cited: 43

Are replication rates the same across academic fields? Community forecasts from the DARPA SCORE programme
Michael Gordon, Domenico Viganola, Michaël Bishop, et al.
Royal Society Open Science (2020) Vol. 7, Iss. 7, pp. 200566-200566
Open Access | Times Cited: 43

Page 1 - Next Page

Scroll to top